http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2009/03/22/AR2009032201663.html?wpisrc=newsletter

Why Righties and Lefties? Scientists Have Hands Full.
By David Brown
Washington Post Staff Writer 
Monday, March 23, 2009; Page A07 

Five of the last seven presidents have been left-handed. Ford, Reagan, Bush the 
elder, Clinton and now Obama (but not Carter or Bush the younger). 

This Story
  a.. Why Righties and Lefties? Scientists Have Hands Full.
  b.. The Lefty Advantage
So what does this mean in a world where only one out of every 10 people, 
roughly speaking, is a lefty? 

The answer is . . . nobody knows. It may be a fluke. But even if it isn't, 
exactly what left-handedness has to do with political skill, intelligence, 
popularity, family connections, wealth and luck -- all at play in our selection 
of national leaders -- is almost certainly a matter of subtle advantage, not 
one of dramatic benefit. 

What is clear is that "handedness" runs all through the animal world. 

Once thought to be uniquely human, some version of this attribute has been seen 
in chimpanzees, marmosets, cats, chickens, toads, mice, rats and almost 
certainly thousands of other species. It is present in animals that don't have 
hands (fish) and in some that don't have backbones (honeybees). 

In biology, this phenomenon is known as "lateralization." It is the preference 
for doing or perceiving things more with one side of the body than the other. 
It appears to be an important -- although perhaps not necessary -- consequence 
of having a brain. 

Like many structures in the body, the brain is "bilaterally symmetrical." It is 
made up of two halves -- called "hemispheres" -- divided by a plane that makes 
one the mirror image of the other. 

Lateralization saves space and, therefore, working capacity, by not requiring 
that both hemispheres do the same thing. It diminishes the chance of 
interference and confusion, which might arise if each side of the brain 
independently analyzed the same input from the environment and came up with its 
own decisions about what to do about it. It also allows the brain to sometimes 
do two things at once. 

"Any brain seems to lateralize if it can," said Lesley J. Rogers, a longtime 
researcher in the field who is an emerita professor at the University of New 
England in Australia. 

The brain's asymmetry is primarily in function, not structure (although careful 
measurement shows that certain regions are bigger on one side than the other in 
nearly everyone). The most dramatic example involves language. 

The ability to produce and comprehend language emanates from the left side of 
the brain in more than 95 percent of right-handed people and in about 70 
percent of left-handed ones. That difference is a big clue that the neural 
wiring -- and perhaps more subtle things -- may be different in lefties. 

Curiously, there is no anatomical "home" for handedness the way there is for 
language. For most voluntary movement, however, each side of the brain controls 
the opposite side of the body. A part of the right side of the brain drives the 
left arm and leg, and vice versa. Similarly, sensation, including vision, that 
is perceived on one side of the body is projected to the opposite side of the 
brain for processing. 

What determines whether a person is right- or left-handed is not really known, 
although it seems to be a combination of genes, environment and culture. 

About 27 percent of sons of left-handed parents are left-handed, compared with 
10 percent of sons of right-handed parents. However, about 20 percent of 
identical twins have different handedness. So genes count for something but not 
everything. 

Because left-handedness is more common in men than in women, many scientists 
have speculated that testosterone has something to do with it. Lots of research 
has been done (and more is underway) testing this hypothesis. 

The best evidence at the moment supports the idea that testosterone may "prune 
back" some of the fibers in the corpus callosum, the huge bundle of nerves that 
connects the right and left halves of the brain across the midline. That, in 
turn, may diminish traffic between the two hemispheres, enhancing 
lateralization of function. 

"The effect seems to be on the strength of lateralization, not on the 
direction," said Ton G.G. Groothuis, who is researching this effect at the 
University of Groningen in the Netherlands. He hastened to add that, as with 
all differences in lateralization, "the size of this effect is not very large." 

Prenatal effects are much clearer in chickens, where lateralization is largely 
determined by which eye is exposed to light coming through the shell during 
incubation. 

In most chick embryos, it's the right eye. That produces adult birds that are 
better at identifying food and prey using the right eye (with the information 
processed on the left side of the brain), and better at detecting predators and 
sexual advances using the left eye (and right side of the brain). 

When eggs are incubated in the dark, however, the chicks hatch with a lesser 
degree of that inborn tendency. If they are then raised together, the whole 
group tends to end up with the same lateralization of behavior, either right or 
left. 

This last observation suggests that social interactions help determine the 
chicken brain's division of labor. Groothuis said this is not surprising in 
species that travel in flocks, for whom simultaneous movement of the flock is a 
form of self-defense. 

In fish, the brain's lateralization also affects individual behavior, at least 
to some extent. 

For example, if you put a mosquitofish in a narrow tank with a caged predator 
at one end, it will inspect the predator more closely if there's a mirror along 
the fish's left side than if there's one along the right. 

In both cases, the mirror makes the fish think it is not alone. But when the 
mirror is on the left, the image is presented to the left eye and sent to the 
right side of the brain, where social interactions are processed better. 

The ultimate effect, Rogers said, is that mosquitofish "are braver when they 
have a companion on the left side." 

Whether the slight differences in brain lateralization between right- and 
left-handed people similarly affects behavior (beyond the use of our hands and 
feet) isn't known. 

A more basic question is: Why does handedness exist? 

Left-handed people are more likely to die from accidents. They may also be more 
likely to have neurological, immunological or psychiatric diseases -- probably 
because at least some left-handedness arises from prenatal damage or birth 
trauma. Whether left-handed people have shorter lives on average is in dispute. 

Despite those "costs," left-handedness has persisted through human evolution. 
One of the reasons is simple: Lefties do better in fights. 

A 2004 study by Charlotte Faurie and Michel Raymond, of the University 
Montpellier II, in France, found that among eight traditional societies, those 
with the highest homicide rates had the highest proportion of left-handed 
people. 

For example, among the Eipo people in the Indonesian province of Irian Jaya, 
where the homicide rate is 3 per 1,000 people per year, 20 percent of people 
are left-handed. Among the Dioula people of Burkina Faso, where the homicide 
rate is 0.01, 3 percent are left-handed. 

This does not mean that lefties are more violent. It means that in violent 
societies, lefties may fare better. 

"When it is important in a society to be a winner of a fight, then left-handers 
have an advantage," Faurie said. 

Of course, there are limits. 

The low prevalence of left-handedness everywhere suggests that the advantage 
holds only when lefties are rare, preserving their unfamiliarity and ability to 
surprise. 

Kirim email ke