-Caveat Lector-

Creating False Memories

Elizabeth F. Loftus
--------------------------------------------------------------http://faculty.w

ashington.edu/eloftus/Articles/sciam.htm-----------------

In 1986 Nadean Cool, a nurse's aide in Wisconsin, sought therapy from a
psychiatrist to help her cope with her reaction to a traumatic event
experienced by her daughter. During therapy, the psychiatrist used hypnosis
and other suggestive techniques to dig out buried memories of abuse that Cool
herself had allegedly experienced. In the process, Cool became convinced that
she had repressed memories of having been in a satanic cult, of eating
babies, of being raped, of having sex with animals and of being forced to
watch the murder of her eight-year-old friend. She came to believe that she
had more than 120 personalities-children, adults, angels and even a duck-all
because, Cool was told, she had experienced severe childhood sexual and
physical abuse. The psychiatrist also performed exorcisms on her, one of
which lasted for five hours and included the sprinkling of holy water and
screams for Satan to leave Cool's body.

When Cool finally realized that false memories had been planted, she sued the
psychiatrist for malpractice. In March 1997, after five weeks of trial, her
case was settled out of court for $2.4 million. Nadean Cool is not the only
patient to develop false memories as a result of questionable therapy. In
Missouri in 1992 a church counselor helped Beth Rutherford to remember during
therapy that her father, a clergyman, had regularly raped her between the
ages of seven and 14 and that her mother sometimes helped him by holding her
down. Under her therapist's guidance, Rutherford developed memories of her
father twice impregnating her and forcing her to abort the fetus herself with
a coat hanger.The father had to resign from his post as a clergyman when the
allegations were made public. Later medical examination of the daughter
revealed, however, that she was still a virgin at age 22 and had never been
pregnant. The daughter sued the therapist and received a $1-million
settlement in 1996.

About a year earlier two juries returned verdicts against a Minnesota
psychiatrist accused of planting false memories by former patients Vynnette
Hamanne and Elizabeth Carlson, who under hypnosis and sodium amytal, and
after being fed misinformation about the workings of memory, had come to
remember horrific abuse by family members. The juries awarded Hammane $2.67
million and Carlson $2.5 million for their ordeals.

In all four cases, the women developed memories about childhood abuse in
therapy and then later denied their authenticity. How can we determine if
memories of childhood abuse are true or false? Without corroboration, it is
very difficult to differentiate between false memories and true ones. Also,
in these cases, some memories were contrary to physical evidence, such as
explicit and detailed recollections of rape and abortion when medical
examination confirmed virginity. How is it possible for people to acquire
elaborate and confident false memories? A growing number of investigations
demonstrate that under the right circumstances false memories can be
instilled rather easily in some people.

My own research into memory distortion goes back to the early 1970s, when I
began studies of the "misinformation effect." These studies show that when
people who witness an event are later exposed to new and misleading
information about it, their recollections often become distorted. In one
example, participants viewed a simulated automobile accident at an
intersection with a stop sign. After the viewing, half the participants
received a suggestion that the traffic sign was a yield sign. When asked
later what traffic sign they remembered seeing at the intersection, those who
had been given the suggestion tended to claim that they had seen a yield
sign. Those who had not received the phony information were much more
accurate in their recollection of the traffic sign.

My students and I have now conducted more than 200 experiments involving over
20,000 individuals that document how exposure to misinformation induces
memory distortion. In these studies, people "recalled" a conspicuous barn in
a bucolic scene that contained no buildings at all, broken glass and tape
recorders that were not in the scenes they viewed, a white instead of a blue
vehicle in a crime scene, and Minnie Mouse when they actually saw Mickey
Mouse. Taken together, these studies show that misinformation can change an
individual's recollection in predictable and sometimes very powerful ways.

Misinformation has the potential for invading our memories when we talk to
other people, when we are suggestively interrogated or when we read or view
media coverage about some event that we may have experienced ourselves. After
more than two decades of exploring the power of misinformation, researchers
have learned a great deal about the conditions that make people susceptible
to memory modification. Memories are more easily modified, for instance, when
the passage of time allows the original memory to fade.



False Childhood Memories
It is one thing to change a detail or two in an otherwise intact memory but
quite another to plant a false memory of an event that never happened. To
study false memory, my students and I first had to find a way to plant a
pseudomemory that would not cause our subjects undue emotional stress, either
in the process of creating the false memory or when we revealed that they had
been intentionally deceived. Yet we wanted to try to plant a memory that
would be at least mildly traumatic, had the experience actually happened.

My research associate, Jacqueline E. Pickrell, and I settled on trying to
plant a specific memory of being lost in a shopping mall or large department
store at about the age of five. Here's how we did it. We asked our subjects,
24 individuals ranging in age from 18 to 53, to try to remember childhood
events that had been recounted to us by a parent, an older sibling or another
close relative. We prepared a booklet for each participant containing
one-paragraph stories about three events that had actually happened to him or
her and one that had not. We constructed the false event using information
about a plausible shopping trip provided by a relative, who also verified
that the participant had not in fact been lost at about the age of five. The
lost-in-the-mall scenario included the following elements: lost for an
extended period, crying, aid and comfort by an elderly woman and, finally,
reunion with the family.


After reading each story in the booklet, the participants wrote what they
remembered about the event. If they did not remember it, they were instructed
to write, "I do not remember this." In two follow-up interviews, we told the
participants that we were interested in examining how much detail they could
remember and how their memories compared with those of their relative. The
event paragraphs were not read to them verbatim, but rather parts were
provided as retrieval cues. The participants recalled something about 49 of
the 72 true events (68 percent) immediately after the initial reading of the
booklet and also in each of the two follow-up interviews. After reading the
booklet, seven of the 24 participants (29 percent) remembered either
partially or fully the false event constructed for them, and in the two
follow-up interviews six participants (25 percent) continued to claim that
they remembered the fictitious event. Statistically, there were some
differences between the true memories and the false ones: participants used
more words to describe the true memories, and they rated the true memories as
being somewhat more clear. But if an onlooker were to observe many of our
participants describe an event, it would be difficult indeed to tell whether
the account was of a true or a false memory. Of course, being lost, however
frightening, is not the same as being abused. But the lost-in-the-mall study
is not about real experiences of being lost; it is about planting false
memories of being lost. The paradigm shows a way of instilling false memories
and takes a step toward allowing us to understand how this might happen in
real-world settings. Moreover, the study provides evidence that people can be
led to remember their past in different ways, and they can even be coaxed
into "remembering" entire events that never happened.

Studies in other laboratories using a similar experimental procedure have
produced similar results. For instance, Ira Hyman, Troy H. Husband and F.
James Billing of Western Washington University asked college students to
recall childhood experiences that had been recounted by their parents. The
researchers told the students that the study was about how people remember
shared experiences differently. In addition to actual events reported by
parents, each participant was given one false event either an overnight
hospitalization for a high fever and a possible ear infection, or a birthday
party with pizza and a clown that supposedly happened at about the age of
five. The parents confirmed that neither of these events actually took place.


Hyman found that students fully or partially recalled 84 percent of the true
events in the first interview and 88 percent in the second interview. None of
the participants recalled the false event during the first interview, but 20
percent said they remembered something about the false event in the second
interview. One participant who had been exposed to the emergency
hospitalization story later remembered a male doctor, a female nurse and a
friend from church who came to visit at the hospital. In another study, along
with true events Hyman presented different false events, such as accidentally
spilling a bowl of punch on the parents of the bride at a wedding reception
or having to evacuate a grocery store when the overhead sprinkler systems
erroneously activated. Again, none of the participants recalled the false
event during the first interview, but 18 percent remembered something about
it in the second interview. For example, during the first interview, one
participant, when asked about the fictitious wedding event, stated, "I have
no clue. I have never heard that one before." In the second interview, the
participant said, "It was an outdoor wedding, and I think we were running
around and knocked something over like the punch bowl or something and made a
big mess and of course got yelled at for it. "

Imagination Inflation
The finding that an external suggestion can lead to the construction of false
childhood memories helps us understand the process by which false memories
arise. It is natural to wonder whether this research is applicable in real
situations such as being interrogated by law officers or in psychotherapy.
Although strong suggestion may not routinely occur in police questioning or
therapy, suggestion in the form of an imagination exercise sometimes does.
For instance, when trying to obtain a confession, law officers may ask a
suspect to imagine having participated in a criminal act. Some mental health
professionals encourage patients to imagine childhood events as a way of
recovering supposedly hidden memories.

Surveys of clinical psychologists reveal that 11 percent instruct their
clients to "let the imagination run wild," and 22 percent tell their clients
to "give free rein to the imagination." Therapist Wendy Maltz, author of a
popular book on childhood sexual abuse, advocates telling the patient: "Spend
time imaging that you were sexually abused, without worrying about accuracy
proving anything, or having your ideas make sense .... Ask yourself ... these
questions: What time of day is it? Where are you? Indoors or outdoors? What
kind of things are happening? Is there one or more person with you?" Maltz
further recommends that therapists continue to ask questions such as "Who
would have been likely perpetrators? When were you most vulnerable to sexual
abuse in your life?"

The increasing use of such imagination exercises led me and several
colleagues to wonder about their consequences. What happens when people
imagine childhood experiences that did not happen to them? Does imagining a
childhood event increase confidence that it occurred? To explore this, we
designed a three-stage procedure. We first asked individuals to indicate the
likelihood that certain events happened to them during their childhood. The
list contains 40 events, each rated on a scale ranging from "definitely did
not happen" to "definitely did happen." Two weeks later we asked the
participants to imagine that they had experienced some of these events.
Different subjects were asked to imagine different events. Sometime later the
participants again were asked to respond to the original list of 40 childhood
events, indicating how likely it was that these events actually happened to
them. Consider one of the imagination exercises. Participants are told to
imagine playing inside at home after school, hearing a strange noise outside,
running toward the window, tripping, falling, reaching out and breaking the
window with their hand. In addition, we asked participants questions such as
"What did you trip on? How did you feel?" In one study 24 percent of the
participants who imagined the broken-window scenario later reported an
increase in confidence that the event had occurred, whereas only 12 percent
of those who were not asked to imagine the incident reported an increase in
the likelihood that it had taken place. We found this "imagination inflation"
effect in each of the eight events that participants were asked to imagine. A
number of possible explanations come to mind. An obvious one is that an act
of imagination simply makes the event seem more familiar and that familiarity
is mistakenly related to childhood memories rather than to the act of
imagination. Such source confusion when a person does not remember the source
of information can be especially acute for the distant experiences of
childhood.

Studies by Lyn Giff and Henry L. Roediger III of Washington University of
recent rather than childhood experiences more directly connect imagined
actions to the construction of false memory. During the initial session, the
researchers instructed participants to perform the stated action, imagine
doing it or just listen to the statement and do nothing else. The actions
were simple ones: knock on the table, lift the stapler, break the toothpick,
cross your fingers, roll your eyes. During the second session, the
participants were asked to imagine some of the actions that they had not
previously performed. During the final session, they answered questions about
what actions they actually performed during the initial session. The
investigators found that the more times participants imagined an unperformed
action, the more likely they were to remember having performed it.

Impossible Memories
It is highly unlikely that an adult can recall genuine episodic memories from
the first year of life, in part because the hippocampus, which plays a key
role in the creation of memories, has not matured enough to form and store
longlasting memories that can be retrieved in adulthood.


A procedure for planting "impossible" memories about experiences that occur
shortly after birth has been developed by the late Nicholas Spanos and his
collaborators at Carleton University. Individuals are led to believe that
they have well-coordinated eye movements and visual exploration skills
probably because they were born in hospitals that hung swinging, colored
mobiles over infant cribs. To confirm whether they had such an experience,
half the participants are hypnotized, age-regressed to the day after birth
and asked what they remembered. The other half of the group participates in a
"guided mnemonic restructuring" procedure that uses age regression as well as
active encouragement to re-create the infant experiences by imagining them..
Spanos and his co-workers found that the vast majority of their subjects were
susceptible to these memory-planting procedures. Both the hypnotic and guided
participants reported infant memories. Surprisingly, the guided group did so
somewhat more (95 versus 70 percent). Both groups remembered the colored
mobile at a relatively high rate (56 percent of the guided group and 46
percent of the hypnotic subjects). Many participants who did not remember the
mobile did recall other things, such as doctors, nurses, bright lights, cribs
and masks. Also, in both groups, of those who reported memories of infancy,
49 percent felt that they were real memories, as opposed to 16 percent who
claimed that they were merely fantasies. These findings confirm earlier
studies that many individuals can be led to construct complex, vivid and
detailed false memories via a rather simple procedure. Hypnosis clearly is
not necessary.



How False Memories Form
In the lost-in-the-mall study, implantation of false memory occurred when
another person, usually a family member, claimed that the incident happened.
Corroboration of an event by another person can be a powerful technique for
instilling a false memory. In fact, merely claiming to have seen a person do
something can lead that person to make a false confession of wrongdoing.
This effect was demonstrated in a study by Saul M. Kassin and his colleagues
at Williams College, who investigated the reactions of individuals falsely
accused of damaging a computer by pressing the wrong key. The innocent
participants initially denied the charge, but when a confederate said that
she had seen them perform the action, many participants signed a confession,
internalized guilt for the act and went on to confabulate details that were
consistent with that belief. These findings show that false incriminating
evidence can induce people to accept guilt for a crime they did not commit
and even to develop memories to support their guilty feelings.
Research is beginning to give us an understanding of how false memories of
complete, emotional and self-participatory experiences are created in adults.
First, there are social demands on individuals to remember; for instance,
researchers exert some pressure on participants in a study to come up with
memories. Second, memory construction by imagining events can be explicitly
encouraged when people are having trouble remembering. And, finally,
individuals can be encouraged not to think about whether their constructions
are real or not. Creation of false memories is most likely to occur when
these external factors are present, whether in an experimental setting, in a
therapeutic setting or during everyday activities.

False memories are constructed by combining actual memories with the content
of suggestions received from others. During the process, individuals may
forget the source of the information. This is a classic example of source
confusion, in which the content and the source become dissociated.
Of course, because we can implant false childhood memories in some
individuals in no way implies that all memories that arise after suggestion
are necessarily false. Put another way, although experimental work on the
creation of false memories may raise doubt about the validity of long-buried
memories, such as repeated trauma, it in no way disproves them. Without
corroboration, there is little that can be done to help even the most
experienced evaluator to differentiate true memories from ones that were
suggestively planted.

The precise mechanisms by which such false memories are constructed await
further research. We still have much to learn about the degree of confidence
and the characteristics of false memories created in these ways, and we need
to discover what types of individuals are particularly susceptible to these
forms of suggestion and who is resistant.

As we continue this work, it is important to heed the cautionary tale in the
data we have already obtained: mental health professionals and others must be
aware of how greatly they can influence the recollection of events and of the
urgent need for maintaining restraint in situations in which imagination is
used as an aid in recovering presumably lost memories.


=============================================================
The Author
ELIZABETH F. LOFTUS is professor of psychology and adjunct professor of law
at the University of Washington. She received her Ph.D. in psychology from
Stanford University in 1970. Her research has focused on human memory,
eyewitness testimony and courtroom procedure. Loftus has published 18 books
and more than 250 scientific articles and has served as an expert witness or
consultant in hundreds of trials, including the McMartin preschool
molestation case. Her book Eyewitness Testimony won a National Media Award
from the American Psychological Foundation. She has received honorary
doctorates from Miami University, Leiden University and John Jay College of
Criminal Justice. Loftus was recently elected president of the American
Psychological Society.



Further Reading
THE MYTH OF REPRESSED MEMORY. Elizabeth F Loftus and Katherine Ketcham. St.
Martin's Press, 1994.
THE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF FALSE CONFESSIONS: COMPLIANCE, INTER NALIZATION, AND
CONFABULATION. Saul M. Kassin and Katherine L. Kiechel in Psychological
Science, Vol. 7, NO. 3, pages 12S-128; May 1996.

IMAGINATION INFLATION: IMAGINING A CHILDHOOD EVENT INFLATES CONFIDENCE THAT
IT OCCURRED. Maryanne Carry, Charles G. Manning, Elizabeth F. Loftus and
Steven J. Sherman in Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, Vol. 3, NO. 2, pages
208-214; June 1996.

REMEMBERING OUR PAST: STUDIES IN AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY. Edited by David C.
Rubin. Cambridge University Press, 1996.

SEARCHING FOR MEMORY: THE BRAIN, THE MIND, AND THE PAST. Daniel L. Schacter.
BasicBooks, 1996.

<A HREF="http://www.ctrl.org/">www.ctrl.org</A>
DECLARATION & DISCLAIMER
==========
CTRL is a discussion & informational exchange list. Proselytizing propagandic
screeds are unwelcomed. Substance—not soap-boxing—please!  These are
sordid matters and 'conspiracy theory'—with its many half-truths, mis-
directions and outright frauds—is used politically by different groups with
major and minor effects spread throughout the spectrum of time and thought.
That being said, CTRLgives no endorsement to the validity of posts, and
always suggests to readers; be wary of what you read. CTRL gives no
credence to Holocaust denial and nazi's need not apply.

Let us please be civil and as always, Caveat Lector.
========================================================================
Archives Available at:
http://peach.ease.lsoft.com/archives/ctrl.html
 <A HREF="http://peach.ease.lsoft.com/archives/ctrl.html">Archives of
[EMAIL PROTECTED]</A>

http:[EMAIL PROTECTED]/
 <A HREF="http:[EMAIL PROTECTED]/">ctrl</A>
========================================================================
To subscribe to Conspiracy Theory Research List[CTRL] send email:
SUBSCRIBE CTRL [to:] [EMAIL PROTECTED]

To UNsubscribe to Conspiracy Theory Research List[CTRL] send email:
SIGNOFF CTRL [to:] [EMAIL PROTECTED]

Om

Reply via email to