Arthur, thank you for sharing this once again. Brian Eno’s essay The Big Here and Long Now @ http://www.paricenter.com/library/papers/eno01.php was
very interesting - and new - to me. “Humans are capable of a unique trick: creating realities by
first imagining them, by experiencing them in their minds. When Martin Luther
King said "I have a dream…", he was inviting others to dream it with
him. Once a dream becomes shared in that way, current reality gets measured
against it and then modified towards it. As soon as we sense the possibility of
a more desirable world, we begin behaving differently – as though that world is
starting to come into existence, as though, in our minds at least, we’re
already there. The dream becomes an invisible force which pulls us forward. By
this process it starts to come true. The act of imagining something makes it
real. This imaginative
process can be seeded and nurtured by artists and designers, for, since the
beginning of the 20th century, artists have been moving away from an idea of
art as something finished, perfect, definitive and unchanging towards a view of
artworks as processes or the seeds for processes – things that exist and change
in time, things that are never finished. Sometimes this is quite explicit - as
in Walter de Maria’s ‘Lightning Field’ – a huge grid of metal poles designed to
attract lightning. Many musical compositions don’t have one form, but change
unrepeatingly over time – many of my own pieces and Jem Finer’s Artangel
installation "LongPlayer" are like this. Artworks in general are
increasingly regarded as seeds – seeds for processes that need a viewer’s (or a
whole culture’s) active mind in which to develop. Increasingly working with
time, culture-makers see themselves as people who start things, not finish
them. And what is possible
in art becomes thinkable in life. We become our new selves first in simulacrum,
through style and fashion and art, our deliberate immersions in virtual worlds.
Through them we sense what it would be like to be another kind of person with
other kinds of values. We rehearse new feelings and sensitivities. We imagine
other ways of thinking about our world and its future.” Following that, read Mary Catherine
Bateson’s interview in Global Business Network, Listening to Change, where she discusses cultural diversity,
globalization and her credo “you are what you are willing to learn” in the face
of constant change, which we are gradually accepting as a positive in society,
she says, not resisting it, as in the past - except for those who want us to
live in the past. She also
presents GW Bush as a “learner” whose pose as such “charms” many Americans,
because he defers to his elders. See
http://www.gbn.org/ArticleDisplayServlet.srv?aid=430
and go to the pdf download. Which
makes this reply to an academic paper’s public airing interesting, given the
debate about conservatism and liberalism here and everywhere else, and the
roots of our differences of opinions. - KWC Political Opinion, Not
Pathology
By Arie W. Kruglanski and John T. Jost, Thursday, August
28, 2003; Page A27 @ http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/articles/A56563-2003Aug27.html In the May issue of
Psychological Bulletin, we published a review that statistically summarizes
dozens of studies conducted over 50 years dealing with psychological
differences associated with left- vs. right-wing thinking. Based on this literature, we found that the likelihood of adopting
conservative rather than liberal political opinions is significantly
correlated, among other psychological dimensions, with a sense of societal
instability, fear of death, intolerance of ambiguity, need for closure, lower
cognitive complexity and a sense of threat. Apparently without
reading our original articles or attempting to contact any of us, many
commentators and syndicated columnists, including Ann Coulter and Cal Thomas --
George Will [op-ed, Aug. 10] apparently read but misunderstood our work) --
assumed that such a psychological analysis of ideology entails a judgment that
conservatism must be abnormal, pathological or even the result of mental
illness. The British media seem to
have settled on the highly stigmatized and equally inaccurate term
"neuroses." All of this
reflects a crude and outdated perception of psychological research. Historically, some of
the better known psychological analyses of right-wing thinking, especially the
famous Adorno et al. volume on "The Authoritarian Personality"
(1950), assumed that anti-Semitism and racial intolerance were consequences of
faulty parenting styles and traumatic childhood experiences. The German psychologist Erich Jantsch
in 1938 had described liberalism as morbid. We part ways with these and other
theories based on a "medical model" that ranks political orientations
on dimensions of abnormality. All
the variables we have reviewed pertain to normal cognitive and motivational
functioning. We would argue that all beliefs have a partial basis in one's
needs, fears and desires, including beliefs that form one's political ideology. Our research has identified several
factors that seem to underlie the propensity to find conservative vs. liberal
thought systems appealing. It's wrong to conclude
that our results provide only bad news for conservatives. True, we find some support for the
traditional "rigidity-of-the-right" hypothesis, but it is also true
that liberals could be characterized on the basis of our overall profile as
relatively disorganized, indecisive and perhaps overly drawn to ambiguity --
all of which may be liabilities in mass politics and other public and
professional domains. Because we
assume that all beliefs (ideological, scientific and otherwise) are partially
(but never completely) determined by one's needs, fears and desires, we see
nothing pathological about this process.
It is simply part of what it means to be human. Our "trade-off" model of
human psychology assumes that any trait or motivation has potential advantages
and disadvantages, depending on the situation. A heightened sensitivity to threat and uncertainty is by no
means maladaptive in all contexts.
Even closed-mindedness may be useful, provided one tends to have a
closed mind about appropriate values and accurate opinions; a reluctance to
abandon one's prior convictions in favor of new fads can be a good thing. The important task for social
scientists is to identify the conditions under which each of these cognitive
and motivational styles is beneficial, rather than touting one or the other as
inherently and invariably superior. Our findings highlight the importance of
situations and historical factors that can produce political shifts by
affecting psychological needs pertaining to uncertainty and threat.
The need to achieve closure and to resolve ambiguity, for example, are
heightened under conditions of destabilizing uncertainty (for example, with the
outbreak of terrorism, economic turmoil or political instability). Thus our research is best understood as
addressing the cognitive and motivational bases of conservatism (and liberalism)
rather than the personalities of conservatives (and liberals). We readily acknowledge
that identifying the motivational underpinnings of a belief system does not
constitute a valid argument in a political debate any more than it does in
scientific debates. What counts is the cogency of the political
arguments and the degree to which they fit with independently verifiable facts
and reasonable assumptions. When the dust settles on the current
debate, we hope that these important messages will be seen as the real focus of
our research. Arie
W. Kruglanski is distinguished university professor of psychology at the
University of Maryland. John T. Jost is an associate professor in Stanford's
Graduate School of Business. This article was written in collaboration with
Jack Glaser and Frank J. Sulloway, both of the University of California at
Berkeley.
|
- [Futurework] FW: Pari Center Newsletter Cordell . Arthur
- RE: [Futurework] FW: Pari Center Newsletter Karen Watters Cole
- RE: [Futurework] FW: Pari Center Newsletter Cordell . Arthur
- Re: [Futurework] FW: Pari Center Newsletter Ray Evans Harrell