Jan 16 2024

Maybe I should have realized it, but I didn't. There are magicians who can
essentially control how a coin they flip lands. It needs practice, like all
good things, but it can be done.

Though if you're not motivated to start practicing that, there's something
else to intrigue you. Flipped coins show a slight, but definite, tendency
to land the way they were before the flip. There's physics that will
explain this, but there's also a team of 48 researchers that flipped coins
a total of 350,757 times and confirmed this tendency.

Take a moment to comprehend that. Over 350K times!

Done? Now read my article (Mint, December 29) and let me know what you
think.

 How magicians control flip of a coin,
https://www.livemint.com/opinion/columns/how-magicians-control-flip-of-a-coin-11703779854563.html

cheers,
dilip

---

How magicians control flip of a coin


Before writing this, I divided 350,757 by 48. The result? Just over 7307.
Which means 48 people each tossed a coin 7307 times. Give that some
meaning: If you sat down and tossed a coin once every second without pause,
it would take you a little over two hours to toss it 7307 times.

Question is, would you want to do this? Probably not. But what if I tell
you it's in the pursuit of science? Then you might change your mind. Maybe
not once every second for two hours without pause, but you might agree to
perform 7000+ tosses. And that's more or less what each of these 48 people
did.

Question is, why did they do it? To answer that, we go back in time a
decade-and-a-half.

In a 2007 paper, the mathematician Persi Diaconis - famed in mathematical
circles for his skills in magic - and two colleagues reported a rather
remarkable finding. "Vigorously flipped coins," they wrote, "tend to come
up the same way they started. ... For natural flips, the chance of coming
up as started is about 0.51." ("Dynamical Bias in the Coin Toss", Persi
Diaconis et al, SIAM Review, 2007,
https://statweb.stanford.edu/~cgates/PERSI/papers/dyn_coin_07.pdf)

That is, if a coin is tails-up when flipped, it has a slightly higher
chance of landing tails, rather than heads - 0.51 to 0.49. Now this is so
slightly higher that it makes no real difference on a single coin toss,
like the one that starts off a tennis or cricket match. But instead, let's
say you have a bet with a friend that depends not on one, but a thousand
tosses. Let's say the bet is simply that when done, you will have called
correctly more often than him. Let's say you can peer closely to see which
face is up before the tosser tosses, and you call that face, every time. In
such an experiment, you're likely to win your bet, because you can expect
to call correctly about 510 times out of 1000.

This is what Diaconis and colleagues concluded. And why this slight
preference for the starting position? They start by referring to a study
that "showed that ... a vigorous flip, caught in the hand without bouncing,
lands heads up half the time." But your garden variety coin toss is not
usually so neat. "Naturally tossed coins obey the laws of mechanics," they
explain, "and their flight is determined by their initial conditions."  The
coins also "precess": the coin's rotation itself changes the nature of that
rotation, as the coin flies through the air. This is just normal. Tops
precess as they rotate. So does our planet Earth. This is why the North
Pole points at the star Polaris today, but pointed at Alpha Draconis about
5000 years ago, and will point at Vega in another 13,000 years.

In the case of the flipped coin, Diaconis and colleagues took into account
its "angular momentum vector" - never mind what that means - and the angle
the vector makes with the surface of the coin itself. I'm simplifying this
somewhat here, but in short, they explain that if that angle is zero,
there's no precession. But a coin is almost never tossed that way. If the
angle is greater than 45°, the coin "wobbles around" but never turns over -
when it is caught in the hand, it shows the face it started with. In fact,
staying true to Diaconis' roots as a magician, the paper notes that
"magicians and gamblers can carry out such controlled flips which appear
visually indistinguishable from normal flips." Meaning that they can
control which way the coin lands. But less accomplished coin tossers, like
me, cannot control that angle and thus the precession. So the coin lands
unpredictably.

>From there, the paper dives into plenty more exotic mathematics. But the
researchers find that in coins flipped naturally, there's enough precession
"to force a bias of at least 0.01."

Going even further from there, Diaconis et al consider another way of using
a coin for random decisions - spinning it rather than tossing it. This can
result in "huge variations" from any expected 50-50 result, attributable to
the shape of the coin's edge and all that's embossed on the coin. In an
experiment at the University of California, Berkeley, students spun the US
1-cent coin, the "penny", 100 times each. These had a noticeable tendency
to come up tails. In fact, some students reported more than 90% tails.

And if that's surprising, think of how we usually toss a coin. It flies
into the air and falls to the ground, sometimes spinning on its edge before
coming to rest. Combine that spin with the effect of precession and we
might have even more variation from 50-50 in how the coins come up.

Anyway: Diaconis' bias is so slight that "to detect [it] would require
250,000 tosses. The classical assumptions of independence with probability
1/2 are pretty solid."

Naturally, this got scientists interested in testing the "counterintuitive
prediction" that a flipped coin lands on the same side as it started with,
51% of the time. František Bartoš of the University of Amsterdam was one
such. He got a group of 48 other researchers to agree to toss coins - 46
different currencies and denominations - recording which face was up before
they tossed, which was up when they caught the coin. Note that there is
film footage of some of these intrepid scientists doing this work (e.g.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=3xNg51mv-fk). In the service of science,
after all.

Between the 48, they tossed coins a total of 350,757 times, "a number that
- to the best of our knowledge – dwarfs all previous efforts." ("Fair Coins
Tend to Land on the Same Side They Started: Evidence from 350,757 Flips",
František Bartoš and 49 others, https://arxiv.org/pdf/2310.04153.pdf)

What was the result of all this diligent tossing?

First, note that their "data show no trace of a heads-tails bias.
Specifically, we obtained 175,420 heads out of 350,757 tosses." Pretty much
exactly 50%.

But remember, the tossers noted the coins' start positions. This confirmed
the Diaconis finding. "The coins landed how they started more often than
50%. Specifically, the data feature 178,078 same-side landings from 350,757
tosses." That's 50.77%. Given the large number of tosses, that's a
significant difference from chance, or 50%. As Bartoš remarked, "If you bet
a dollar on the outcome of a coin toss 1000 times, knowing the starting
position of the coin toss would earn you US$19 on average."

Admittedly, not the best way to make a living. Still, please don't let the
tosser cover the coin as she tosses.

-- 
My book with Joy Ma: "The Deoliwallahs"
Twitter: @DeathEndsFun
Death Ends Fun: http://dcubed.blogspot.com

-- 
You received this message because you are subscribed to the Google Groups 
"Dilip's essays" group.
To unsubscribe from this group and stop receiving emails from it, send an email 
to dilips-essays+unsubscr...@googlegroups.com.
To view this discussion on the web, visit 
https://groups.google.com/d/msgid/dilips-essays/CAEiMe8oULqU-xtQtgvVP0Mc_%3DgBUJ%3DLh3wb4hT24EphfvRwhaQ%40mail.gmail.com.

Reply via email to