[tips] Hero Worship and the Confirmation Bias

2009-09-11 Thread Mike Palij
On this day of reflection I have been thinking about whom we as a
culture and society consider to be heros and how we interpret who they
are and what they have done.  Although this might have relevance to
the event of 9/11/01 I am actually thinking about psychologists who
are held in high esteem and presented as a type of hero for other
psychologists to be proud of and as an example to be emulated.
One can think of many examples, old and recent, of people who
are seen as having made a significant contribution in some form,
for example, William James is often spoken as a founding father
of American psychology but this is usually done with a selective
presentation of his activities and beliefs, that is, those activities
of James that psychologists might be proud of are promoted and
emphasized that those activities and beliefs that they may be embarassd
by, such as his belief in spiritualism and his promotion of it, not so
much.  In the case of Sir Ronald Fisher, Stephen Jay Gould tried
to reconcile the apparent genius of his contribution to statistics
and genetics with his sincere beliefs in and promotion of eugenics
as a solution of society's problems (i.e., promoting scientific
racism).  Are they comparable cases involving psychologists?

The question then arise:  Why to psychologists, especially teachers,
seem to engage in a form of the confirmation bias in presenting 
psychologists whom someone or group has considered significant?
(for some backgorund on the confirmation bias see the wikipedia
entry but there is large literature on this; I would also suggest
looking at Ray Nickerson's 1998 article in the Review of General
Psychology: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Confirmation_bias )
If we start from the assumption that all people are imperfect, that
they cannot consistently engage in good and/or ethical behavior,
then why instructors emphasize only one side (the good or the bad)
while they should be attempting to apply their own critical thinking
abilities to both the work of a psychologists and that person's
character.

I wonder, for example, if a psychologist, say dates undergraduates
and even goes on to marry them, is this an ethical breach and if so
is it a minor or major offense?  How does engaging in such behavior 
affect that psychologist's ability to serve as a source of ethical guidance?
I see one case like this that seems to popping up in a number of
contexts and been concerned with how to interpert this.  Anyone
have any thoughts on this?

-Mike Palij
New York University
m...@nyu.edu




---
To make changes to your subscription contact:

Bill Southerly (bsouthe...@frostburg.edu)


Re: [tips] Hero Worship and the Confirmation Bias

2009-09-11 Thread Michael Smith
Well, I wouldn't consider any psychologist past or present a hero,
nor am I proud of any.
I also usually try to introduce a life context to the particular
person's contribution. This is usually enough to dispel any hero
worship


I also find it a peculiar idea that a psychologist serves as a source
of ethical guidance.

Fortunately I think most ordinary people don't consider psychologists
much at all, and if they are looking for ethical guidance they will go
to their friends, priests, ministers, and rabbi's. A far better
choice.

--Mike

---
To make changes to your subscription contact:

Bill Southerly (bsouthe...@frostburg.edu)


Re: [tips] Hero Worship and the Confirmation Bias

2009-09-11 Thread Christopher D. Green
Mike Palij wrote:
 ... I am actually thinking about psychologists who
 are held in high esteem and presented as a type of hero for other
 psychologists to be proud of and as an example to be emulated.
 One can think of many examples, old and recent, of people who
 are seen as having made a significant contribution in some form,
 for example, William James is often spoken as a founding father
 of American psychology but this is usually done with a selective
 presentation of his activities and beliefs, that is, those activities
 of James that psychologists might be proud of are promoted and
 emphasized that those activities and beliefs that they may be embarassd
 by, such as his belief in spiritualism and his promotion of it, not so
 much.  

Indeed. I found that Louis Menand's _The Metaphysical Club_ was a 
wonderful antidote to William James hero-worship. And he does it without 
bashing James so much as just showing how his mental foibles (esp. his 
chronic indecisiveness) were as much a part of his philosophy as were 
his mental strengths.

 In the case of Sir Ronald Fisher, Stephen Jay Gould tried
 to reconcile the apparent genius of his contribution to statistics
 and genetics with his sincere beliefs in and promotion of eugenics
 as a solution of society's problems (i.e., promoting scientific
 racism).  Are they comparable cases involving psychologists?
   

A lot of them. For instance, James McKeen Cattell was a strong 
eugenicist. Indeed, the reason he opposed conscription so adamantly 
during WWI (so adamantly that it got him fired from Columbia, to a first 
approximation), was based on his belief that it would result in a high 
kill-off of genetically superior youth, thereby undermining the American 
gene pool in the long term. The case of Terman is well-known too. 
Lashley and Watson were both deeply (though not often publicly) racist. 
The only prominent early American psychologist I can think of who 
strongly and publicly opposed eugenics was John Dewey.  By the way, Karl 
Pearson so admired the Germans that he changed the spelling of his first 
name (from Carl).
 The question then arise:  Why to psychologists, especially teachers,
 seem to engage in a form of the confirmation bias in presenting 
 psychologists whom someone or group has considered significant?
   

This is an critical issue for historians of science, who are chronically 
horrified by what typically passes for historical scholarship among 
scientists. The problem is, of course, that when scientists turn their 
hand to writing about the past of their discipline, they almost always 
do it in an intellectualist and celebratory mode. That is, first, the 
main aim is to cover the intellectual developments, but little else 
(except perhaps a bit of mythologized biography -- like Newton's falling 
apple), because, as they like to say, it is the *ideas* (usually the 
discoveries) that matter, not whether the scientist was married or 
single, liberal or conservative, good or evil, etc. But it also serves 
to hide away aspects of a scientist's life that might not seem so 
praiseworth today (such as, e.g.,  Newton's alchemical and religious 
obessions, Darwin's apparent hypochondria, James' spiritualism, or [to 
take a more recent example] R. B. Cattell's white supremacism). Second, 
scientists who write history almost always do it in order to inspire 
younger scientists to emulate the greats of times past, so they extol 
(and often exaggerate) the singular nature of their genius. That is to 
say, when a scientist tries to write history it is often more of a 
moral exercise than a scholarly one.

Over the past 25 years or so, history of science has become a more or 
less independent discipline, conducted mostly by professional 
historians, rather than by scientists. And historians traditionally pay 
great attention to the contexts (intellectual, personal, social, 
cultural, political) in which various scientific ideas (among other 
events) arise. But in the process, modern historians of science have 
alienated a lot scientists who have mistaken their activities for having 
the primary aim of criticizing scientists and science itself (where not 
being sufficiently adulatory counts is regarded as being overly 
critical). This misunderstanding of intentions precipitated the very 
nasty Science Wars of the 1990s. Thankfully, we are mostly over the 
worst of that silliness now, but it still rears its ugly head from time 
to time.

Chris
-- 

Christopher D. Green
Department of Psychology
York University
Toronto, ON M3J 1P3
Canada

 

416-736-2100 ex. 66164
chri...@yorku.ca
http://www.yorku.ca/christo/

==


---
To make changes to your subscription contact:

Bill Southerly (bsouthe...@frostburg.edu)