http://www.nytimes.com/2001/07/21/arts/dailyarts/21LEWI.html

JUL 21, 2001

Damaged Brains and the Death Penalty

By LAURA MANSNERUS

You don't have to be a psychiatrist, Dr. Dorothy Otnow Lewis says, to
know that something was terribly wrong with Ricky Ray Rector, who before
his execution in Arkansas ordered his last meal and asked that the pecan
pie be set aside so he could have it later.

But Dr. Lewis is a psychiatrist, and the Ricky Ray Rector story makes a
point that she has spent many years documenting: the worst criminals are
not a very crafty lot. Almost without exception, Dr. Lewis has found in
evaluating dozens of death-row inmates, they have damaged brains. Most
were also the victims of vicious batterings and often sexual abuse as
children. Psychotic symptoms, especially paranoia, are common.

A professor of psychiatry at New York University, Dr. Lewis is among a
handful of researchers who are rethinking the etiology of violence. Her
studies focus on some of the most violent criminals; she has interviewed
150 to 200 murderers, sorting through their medical histories and, as
much as it can be done, their brains.

Dr. Lewis "has revolutionized the way people think about criminal
behavior," said Elyn R. Saks, who teaches forensic psychiatry at the
University of Southern California Law School.

And while no revolution is at hand in the criminal justice system, legal
scholars say new findings on brain dysfunction are finally gaining
attention, at least where they matter most: in death penalty cases. Just
this year, 4 states banned executions of the mentally retarded, bringing
to 17 the number of the 38 death-penalty states that have made that
exception, and the Supreme Court will hear arguments in one such case
this fall.

Some of the stories Dr. Lewis has heard are told in her 1998 book,
"Guilty by Reason of Insanity." Other stories emerge through raw data in
articles published over 15 years in medical journals. Her latest
article, a study of murderers who were adopted, was accepted this month
by the Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, though
until publication, Dr. Lewis says, she cannot discuss her findings.

Her longtime collaborator, Dr. Jonathan H. Pincus, the chief of
neurology at the Veterans Administration Hospital in Washington, sets
out the neurologist's perspective in his book, "Base Instincts: What
Makes Killers Kill?," published last month.

Dr. Pincus administered the neurological examinations, from simple
reflex tests to EEG's and brain scans, that supplemented the interviews.
The researchers also combed whatever medical records they could find.

In 1986 Dr. Lewis and Dr. Pincus published a study of 15 death row
inmates that found all had suffered severe head injuries in childhood
and about half had been injured by assaults. Six were chronically
psychotic. Far from invoking an "abuse excuse," Dr. Lewis said, all but
one had minimized or denied their psychiatric disorders, figuring that
it was better to be bad than crazy.

Many, she said, had been so traumatized that they could not remember how
they had received their scars. The answers had to come from childhood
medical records and interviews with family members.

In another study, of 14 juveniles sentenced to death, the researchers
found that all had suffered head trauma, most in car accidents but many
by beatings as well. Twelve had suffered brutal physical abuse, five of
those sodomized by relatives.

No one suggests that abuse or brain damage makes a murderer, but Dr.
Lewis says that while most damaged people do not turn into killers,
almost every killer is a damaged person. She concludes that most
murderers are shaped by the combination of damage to the brain,
particularly to the frontal lobes, which control aggression and
impulsiveness, and the even more complex damage visited by repeated,
violent child abuse.

These findings, Dr. Lewis says, cast doubt on legal definitions of
insanity. Many legal experts agree, while others say the law should be
in no hurry to apply new theories in the debate, older than Western
thought itself, between free will and determinism. Many psychiatrists
and psychologists, too, see evil and con artistry where researchers like
Dr. Lewis see disease.

Barbara R. Kirwin, a forensic psychologist who recounted her
examinations of violent murderers in her book, "The Mad, the Bad and the
Innocent," questions Dr. Lewis's studies because, like many medical
studies with small samples, they are not controlled. And if unusual
brain activity can be interpreted, Dr. Kirwin said, "I want to find out
what subcortical firing Mother Theresa has."

Dr. Kirwin's findings on the incidence of child abuse among homicide
defendants differ wildly from Dr. Lewis's. Dr. Kirwin estimates that of
the 300 or so defendants she has studied, 10 percent have been abused,
or "about what you'd find in the general population."

One way of stating their differences is that Dr. Lewis says she has
never seen a "mere sociopath" — that is, someone with a normally
competent brain who simply has a gross lack of empathy — while Dr.
Kirwin says she has seen plenty.

Dr. Lewis has been cited by the Supreme Court at least three times,
along with Dr. Pincus and another longtime colleague, Catherine Yeager,
a senior research assistant at New York University. Dr. Lewis is most
pleased by the citation in a 1991 opinion by Justice Thurgood Marshall,
a dissent in the decision allowing the execution of the brain-damaged
Ricky Ray Rector.

The court has ruled out executions of the insane, though critics say the
legal standard is low: an inmate who is aware of his pending execution
and the reason for it is deemed mentally competent.

Richard E. Redding, a University of Virginia law professor and associate
director of the university's Institute of Law, Psychiatry and Public
Policy, said that when courts overturned death sentences they rarely
addressed a defendant's mental state directly. But he said they were
increasingly receptive to arguments that a defense lawyer's failure to
present evidence of brain damage during the sentencing phase can amount
to ineffective assistance of counsel, which is a ground for reversing a
death sentence.

In that way, Mr. Redding said, research on brain dysfunction, especially
damage to the frontal lobe, "is actually having an effect on real-life
cases."

In a 1999 case that overturned the death sentence of an inmate with a
history of seizures and paranoia, the Illinois Supreme Court found that
his lawyer failed to investigate obvious signs of his irrationality,
which the court said was exemplified by his attempt to dispose of a body
in a dresser drawer. (The expert witness who pointed that out in court
was Dr. Pincus.)

Dr. Lewis and her colleagues study savage and bizarre murders, which she
says are almost by definition the most crazed. In capital cases, Dr.
Lewis says, the elaborate balancing of aggravating and mitigating
factors — those that may be taken into account by judge or jury —
actually frustrates the inquiry because "the grisliness of the crime is
in proportion to the craziness of the act."

However crazy their acts, very few defendants qualify for the classic
insanity defense. For purposes of determining sanity, the test is
whether the defendant knew what he was doing and knew it was wrong,
although some states also require that the defendant be capable of
"conforming his conduct" to the law.

"Responsibility is so wedded into centuries of tradition," said Deborah
W. Denno, a Fordham University law professor who is working on an
article about psychological research on consciousness and its influence
on defining degrees of culpability.

Long before anything was known about wayward neurotransmitters and
frontal lobe lesions, legal theorists referred to free will as a
fiction. The legal scholar Herbert Packer wrote in 1968 that "the law
treats man's conduct as autonomous and willed, not because it is, but
because it is desirable to proceed as if it were."

To Ms. Denno, such thinking is outdated. "Unconscious thought is more
important than we ever thought," she said. "I'm suggesting that the
criminal law is way out of line with what constitutes conscious thought.
There's this dichotomy in criminal law: either you're responsible or
you're not. If you're a sleepwalker or something like that, you're not
held responsible at all."

Dr. Lewis, like Ms. Denno, focuses not on guilt but on punishment, and
she typically works for defendants who ask only the dispensation of life
in prison. "Most of the people I see I would not want running around
again," she said.

Now, in addition to the murderers from adoptive homes, she and her
colleagues are studying serial killers, children who killed their
parents and capital defendants who represent themselves. "Then," she
said, "it's up to the public who they want to kill."

Copyright 2001 The New York Times Company | Privacy Information

--
Jeffry P. Ricker, Ph.D.          Office Phone:  (480) 423-6213
9000 E. Chaparral Rd.            FAX Number: (480) 423-6298
Psychology Department            [EMAIL PROTECTED]
Scottsdale Community College
Scottsdale, AZ  85256-2626

Listowner: Psychologists Educating Students to Think Skeptically (PESTS)

http://www.sc.maricopa.edu/sbscience/pests/index.html


Reply via email to