Linux-Advocacy Digest #698, Volume #32            Thu, 8 Mar 01 01:13:07 EST

Contents:
  Re: What does IQ measure? (The Danimal)

----------------------------------------------------------------------------

Date: Thu, 08 Mar 2001 00:22:12 -0500
From: The Danimal <[EMAIL PROTECTED]>
Crossposted-To: 
alt.destroy.microsoft,comp.os.ms-windows.advocacy,comp.os.ms-windows.nt.advocacy,soc.singles
Subject: Re: What does IQ measure?

CR Lyttle wrote:
> 
> Donovan Rebbechi wrote:
> >
> > I'll butt in here and say that IQ is certainly not a terribly reliable measure
> > of anything, but it's true that it correlates with a "intelligence", no matter
> > how you define "intelligence".
> >
> So which has higher intelligence, an oak forest, or amoebae? Trees do
> communicate, and forest do learn.

Let's have them play chess.

A considerably better read than this thread appeared recently 
in Scientific American:

  http://www.sciam.com/specialissues/1198intelligence/1198gottfred.html
  The General Intelligence Factor

  Despite some popular assertions,[*] a single factor for intelligence, 
  called g, can be measured with IQ tests and does predict success in life 

  by Linda S. Gottfredson

  [*hi jackie---this repost is for you]

  No subject in psychology has provoked more intense public controversy 
  than the study of human intelligence. From its beginning, research on 
  how and why people differ in overall mental ability has fallen prey 
  to political and social agendas that obscure or distort even the most 
  well-established scientific findings. Journalists, too, often present 
  a view of intelligence research that is exactly the opposite of what 
  most intelligence experts believe. For these and other reasons, 
  public understanding of intelligence falls far short of public 
  concern about it. The IQ experts discussing their work in the public 
  arena can feel as though they have fallen down the rabbit hole into 
  Alice's Wonderland.

  The debate over intelligence and intelligence testing focuses on the 
  question of whether it is useful or meaningful to evaluate people 
  according to a single major dimension of cognitive competence. Is 
  there indeed a general mental ability we commonly call 
  "intelligence," and is it important in the practical affairs of life? 
  The answer, based on decades of intelligence research, is an 
  unequivocal yes. No matter their form or content, tests of mental 
  skills invariably point to the existence of a global factor that 
  permeates all aspects of cognition. And this factor seems to have 
  considerable influence on a person's practical quality of life. 
  Intelligence as measured by IQ tests is the single most effective 
  predictor known of individual performance at school and on the job. 
  It also predicts many other aspects of well-being, including a 
  person's chances of divorcing, dropping out of high school, being 
  unemployed or having illegitimate children [see illustration].

  By now the vast majority of intelligence researchers take these 
  findings for granted. Yet in the press and in public debate, the 
  facts are typically dismissed, downplayed or ignored. This 
  misrepresentation reflects a clash between a deeply felt ideal and a 
  stubborn reality. The ideal, implicit in many popular critiques of 
  intelligence research, is that all people are born equally able and 
  that social inequality results only from the exercise of unjust 
  privilege. The reality is that Mother Nature is no egalitarian. 
  People are in fact unequal in intellectual potential--and they are 
  born that way, just as they are born with different potentials for 
  height, physical attractiveness, artistic flair, athletic prowess and 
  other traits. Although subsequent experience shapes this potential, 
  no amount of social engineering can make individuals with widely 
  divergent mental aptitudes into intellectual equals.

  Of course, there are many kinds of talent, many kinds of mental 
  ability and many other aspects of personality and character that 
  influence a person's chances of happiness and success. The functional 
  importance of general mental ability in everyday life, however, means 
  that without onerous restrictions on individual liberty, differences 
  in mental competence are likely to result in social inequality. This 
  gulf between equal opportunity and equal outcomes is perhaps what 
  pains Americans most about the subject of intelligence. The public 
  intuitively knows what is at stake: when asked to rank personal 
  qualities in order of desirability, people put intelligence second 
  only to good health. But with a more realistic approach to the 
  intellectual differences between people, society could better 
  accommodate these differences and minimize the inequalities they 
  create.

  Extracting g

  Early in the century-old study of intelligence, researchers 
  discovered that all tests of mental ability ranked individuals in 
  about the same way. Although mental tests are often designed to 
  measure specific domains of cognition--verbal fluency, say, or 
  mathematical skill, spatial visualization or memory--people who do 
  well on one kind of test tend to do well on the others, and people 
  who do poorly generally do so across the board. This overlap, or 
  intercorrelation, suggests that all such tests measure some global 
  element of intellectual ability as well as specific cognitive skills. 
  In recent decades, psychologists have devoted much effort to 
  isolating that general factor, which is abbreviated g, from the other 
  aspects of cognitive ability gauged in mental tests.

  The statistical extraction of g is performed by a technique called 
  factor analysis. Introduced at the turn of the century by British 
  psychologist Charles Spearman, factor analysis determines the minimum 
  number of underlying dimensions necessary to explain a pattern of 
  correlations among measurements. A general factor suffusing all tests 
  is not, as is sometimes argued, a necessary outcome of factor 
  analysis. No general factor has been found in the analysis of 
  personality tests, for example; instead the method usually yields at 
  least five dimensions (neuroticism, extraversion, conscientiousness, 
  agreeableness and openness to ideas), each relating to different 
  subsets of tests. But, as Spearman observed, a general factor does 
  emerge from analysis of mental ability tests, and leading 
  psychologists, such as Arthur R. Jensen of the University of 
  California at Berkeley and John B. Carroll of the University of North 
  Carolina at Chapel Hill, have confirmed his findings in the decades 
  since. Partly because of this research, most intelligence experts now 
  use g as the working definition of intelligence.

  The general factor explains most differences among individuals in 
  performance on diverse mental tests. This is true regardless of what 
  specific ability a test is meant to assess, regardless of the test's 
  manifest content (whether words, numbers or figures) and regardless 
  of the way the test is administered (in written or oral form, to an 
  individual or to a group). Tests of specific mental abilities do 
  measure those abilities, but they all reflect g to varying degrees as 
  well. Hence, the g factor can be extracted from scores on any diverse 
  battery of tests.

  Conversely, because every mental test is "contaminated" by the 
  effects of specific mental skills, no single test measures only g. 
  Even the scores from IQ tests--which usually combine about a dozen 
  subtests of specific cognitive skills--contain some "impurities" that 
  reflect those narrower skills. For most purposes, these impurities 
  make no practical difference, and g and IQ can be used 
  interchangeably. But if they need to, intelligence researchers can 
  statistically separate the g component of IQ. The ability to isolate 
  g has revolutionized research on general intelligence, because it has 
  allowed investigators to show that the predictive value of mental 
  tests derives almost entirely from this global factor rather than 
  from the more specific aptitudes measured by intelligence tests.

  In addition to quantifying individual differences, tests of mental 
  abilities have also offered insight into the meaning of intelligence 
  in everyday life. Some tests and test items are known to correlate 
  better with g than others do. In these items the "active ingredient" 
  that demands the exercise of g seems to be complexity. More complex 
  tasks require more mental manipulation, and this manipulation of 
  information--discerning similarities and inconsistencies, drawing 
  inferences, grasping new concepts and so on--constitutes intelligence 
  in action. Indeed, intelligence can best be described as the ability 
  to deal with cognitive complexity.

  This description coincides well with lay perceptions of intelligence. 
  The g factor is especially important in just the kind of behaviors 
  that people usually associate with "smarts": reasoning, problem 
  solving, abstract thinking, quick learning. And whereas g itself 
  describes mental aptitude rather than accumulated knowledge, a 
  person's store of knowledge tends to correspond with his or her g 
  level, probably because that accumulation represents a previous 
  adeptness in learning and in understanding new information. The g 
  factor is also the one attribute that best distinguishes among 
  persons considered gifted, average or retarded.

  Several decades of factor-analytic research on mental tests have 
  confirmed a hierarchical model of mental abilities. The evidence, 
  summarized most effectively in Carroll's 1993 book, Human Cognitive 
  Abilities, puts g at the apex in this model, with more specific 
  aptitudes arrayed at successively lower levels: the so-called group 
  factors, such as verbal ability, mathematical reasoning, spatial 
  visualization and memory, are just below g, and below these are 
  skills that are more dependent on knowledge or experience, such as 
  the principles and practices of a particular job or profession.

  Some researchers use the term "multiple intelligences" to label these 
  sets of narrow capabilities and achievements. Psychologist Howard 
  Gardner of Harvard University, for example, has postulated that eight 
  relatively autonomous "intelligences" are exhibited in different 
  domains of achievement. He does not dispute the existence of g but 
  treats it as a specific factor relevant chiefly to academic 
  achievement and to situations that resemble those of school. Gardner 
  does not believe that tests can fruitfully measure his proposed 
  intelligences; without tests, no one can at present determine whether 
  the intelligences are indeed independent of g (or each other). 
  Furthermore, it is not clear to what extent Gardner's intelligences 
  tap personality traits or motor skills rather than mental aptitudes.

  Other forms of intelligence have been proposed; among them, emotional 
  intelligence and practical intelligence are perhaps the best known. 
  They are probably amalgams either of intellect and personality or of 
  intellect and informal experience in specific job or life settings, 
  respectively. Practical intelligence like "street smarts," for 
  example, seems to consist of the localized knowledge and know-how 
  developed with untutored experience in particular everyday settings 
  and activities--the so-called school of hard knocks. In contrast, 
  general intelligence is not a form of achievement, whether local or 
  renowned. Instead the g factor regulates the rate of learning: it 
  greatly affects the rate of return in knowledge to instruction and 
  experience but cannot substitute for either.

  The Biology of g

  Some critics of intelligence research maintain that the notion of 
  general intelligence is illusory: that no such global mental capacity 
  exists and that apparent "intelligence" is really just a by-product 
  of one's opportunities to learn skills and information valued in a 
  particular cultural context. True, the concept of intelligence and 
  the way in which individuals are ranked according to this criterion 
  could be social artifacts. But the fact that g is not specific to any 
  particular domain of knowledge or mental skill suggests that g is 
  independent of cultural content, including beliefs about what 
  intelligence is. And tests of different social groups reveal the same 
  continuum of general intelligence. This observation suggests either 
  that cultures do not construct g or that they construct the same g. 
  Both conclusions undercut the social artifact theory of intelligence.

  Moreover, research on the physiology and genetics of g has uncovered 
  biological correlates of this psychological phenomenon. In the past 
  decade, studies by teams of researchers in North America and Europe 
  have linked several attributes of the brain to general intelligence. 
  After taking into account gender and physical stature, brain size as 
  determined by magnetic resonance imaging is moderately correlated 
  with IQ (about 0.4 on a scale of 0 to 1). So is the speed of nerve 
  conduction. The brains of bright people also use less energy during 
  problem solving than do those of their less able peers. And various 
  qualities of brain waves correlate strongly (about 0.5 to 0.7) with 
  IQ: the brain waves of individuals with higher IQs, for example, 
  respond more promptly and consistently to simple sensory stimuli such 
  as audible clicks. These observations have led some investigators to 
  posit that differences in g result from differences in the speed and 
  efficiency of neural processing. If this theory is true, 
  environmental conditions could influence g by modifying brain 
  physiology in some manner.

  Studies of so-called elementary cognitive tasks (ECTs), conducted by 
  Jensen and others, are bridging the gap between the psychological and 
  the physiological aspects of g. These mental tasks have no obvious 
  intellectual content and are so simple that adults and most children 
  can do them accurately in less than a second. In the most basic 
  reaction-time tests, for example, the subject must react when a light 
  goes on by lifting her index finger off a home button and immediately 
  depressing a response button. Two measurements are taken: the number 
  of milliseconds between the illumination of the light and the 
  subject's release of the home button, which is called decision time, 
  and the number of milliseconds between the subject's release of the 
  home button and pressing of the response button, which is called 
  movement time.

  In this task, movement time seems independent of intelligence, but 
  the decision times of higher-IQ subjects are slightly faster than 
  those of people with lower IQs. As the tasks are made more complex, 
  correlations between average decision times and IQ increase. These 
  results further support the notion that intelligence equips 
  individuals to deal with complexity and that its influence is greater 
  in complex tasks than in simple ones.

  The ECT-IQ correlations are comparable for all IQ levels, ages, 
  genders and racial-ethnic groups tested. Moreover, studies by Philip 
  A. Vernon of the University of Western Ontario and others have shown 
  that the ECT-IQ overlap results almost entirely from the common g 
  factor in both measures. Reaction times do not reflect differences in 
  motivation or strategy or the tendency of some individuals to rush 
  through tests and daily tasks--that penchant is a personality trait. 
  They actually seem to measure the speed with which the brain 
  apprehends, integrates and evaluates information. Research on ECTs 
  and brain physiology has not yet identified the biological 
  determinants of this processing speed. These studies do suggest, 
  however, that g is as reliable and global a phenomenon at the neural 
  level as it is at the level of the complex information processing 
  required by IQ tests and everyday life.

  The existence of biological correlates of intelligence does not 
  necessarily mean that intelligence is dictated by genes. Decades of 
  genetics research have shown, however, that people are born with 
  different hereditary potentials for intelligence and that these 
  genetic endowments are responsible for much of the variation in 
  mental ability among individuals. Last spring an international team 
  of scientists headed by Robert Plomin of the Institute of Psychiatry 
  in London announced the discovery of the first gene linked to 
  intelligence. Of course, genes have their effects only in interaction 
  with environments, partly by enhancing an individual's exposure or 
  sensitivity to formative experiences. Differences in general 
  intelligence, whether measured as IQ or, more accurately, as g are 
  both genetic and environmental in origin--just as are all other 
  psychological traits and attitudes studied so far, including 
  personality, vocational interests and societal attitudes. This is old 
  news among the experts. The experts have, however, been startled by 
  more recent discoveries.

  One is that the heritability of IQ rises with age--that is to say, 
  the extent to which genetics accounts for differences in IQ among 
  individuals increases as people get older. Studies comparing 
  identical and fraternal twins, published in the past decade by a 
  group led by Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr., of the University of Minnesota 
  and other scholars, show that about 40 percent of IQ differences 
  among preschoolers stems from genetic differences but that 
  heritability rises to 60 percent by adolescence and to 80 percent by 
  late adulthood. With age, differences among individuals in their 
  developed intelligence come to mirror more closely their genetic 
  differences. It appears that the effects of environment on 
  intelligence fade rather than grow with time. In hindsight, perhaps 
  this should have come as no surprise. Young children have the 
  circumstances of their lives imposed on them by parents, schools and 
  other agents of society, but as people get older they become more 
  independent and tend to seek out the life niches that are most 
  congenial to their genetic proclivities.

  A second big surprise for intelligence experts was the discovery that 
  environments shared by siblings have little to do with IQ. Many 
  people still mistakenly believe that social, psychological and 
  economic differences among families create lasting and marked 
  differences in IQ. Behavioral geneticists refer to such environmental 
  effects as "shared" because they are common to siblings who grow up 
  together. Research has shown that although shared environments do 
  have a modest influence on IQ in childhood, their effects dissipate 
  by adolescence. The IQs of adopted children, for example, lose all 
  resemblance to those of their adoptive family members and become more 
  like the IQs of the biological parents they have never known. Such 
  findings suggest that siblings either do not share influential 
  aspects of the rearing environment or do not experience them in the 
  same way. Much behavioral genetics research currently focuses on the 
  still mysterious processes by which environments make members of a 
  household less alike.

  g on the Job

  Although the evidence of genetic and physiological correlates of g 
  argues powerfully for the existence of global intelligence, it has 
  not quelled the critics of intelligence testing. These skeptics argue 
  that even if such a global entity exists, it has no intrinsic 
  functional value and becomes important only to the extent that people 
  treat it as such: for example, by using IQ scores to sort, label and 
  assign students and employees. Such concerns over the proper use of 
  mental tests have prompted a great deal of research in recent 
  decades. This research shows that although IQ tests can indeed be 
  misused, they measure a capability that does in fact affect many 
  kinds of performance and many life outcomes, independent of the 
  tests' interpretations or applications. Moreover, the research shows 
  that intelligence tests measure the capability equally well for all 
  native-born English-speaking groups in the U.S.

  If we consider that intelligence manifests itself in everyday life as 
  the ability to deal with complexity, then it is easy to see why it 
  has great functional or practical importance. Children, for example, 
  are regularly exposed to complex tasks once they begin school. 
  Schooling requires above all that students learn, solve problems and 
  think abstractly. That IQ is quite a good predictor of differences in 
  educational achievement is therefore not surprising. When scores on 
  both IQ and standardized achievement tests in different subjects are 
  averaged over several years, the two averages correlate as highly as 
  different IQ tests from the same individual do. High-ability students 
  also master material at many times the rate of their low-ability 
  peers. Many investigations have helped quantify this discrepancy. For 
  example, a 1969 study done for the U.S. Army by the Human Resources 
  Research Office found that enlistees in the bottom fifth of the 
  ability distribution required two to six times as many teaching 
  trials and prompts as did their higher-ability peers to attain 
  minimal proficiency in rifle assembly, monitoring signals, combat 
  plotting and other basic military tasks. Similarly, in school 
  settings the ratio of learning rates between "fast" and "slow" 
  students is typically five to one.

  The scholarly content of many IQ tests and their strong correlations 
  with educational success can give the impression that g is only a 
  narrow academic ability. But general mental ability also predicts job 
  performance, and in more complex jobs it does so better than any 
  other single personal trait, including education and experience. The 
  army's Project A, a seven-year study conducted in the 1980s to 
  improve the recruitment and training process, found that general 
  mental ability correlated strongly with both technical proficiency 
  and soldiering in the nine specialties studied, among them infantry, 
  military police and medical specialist. Research in the civilian 
  sector has revealed the same pattern. Furthermore, although the 
  addition of personality traits such as conscientiousness can help 
  hone the prediction of job performance, the inclusion of specific 
  mental aptitudes such as verbal fluency or mathematical skill rarely 
  does. The predictive value of mental tests in the work arena stems 
  almost entirely from their measurement of g, and that value rises 
  with the complexity and prestige level of the job.

  Half a century of military and civilian research has converged to 
  draw a portrait of occupational opportunity along the IQ continuum. 
  Individuals in the top 5 percent of the adult IQ distribution (above 
  IQ 125) can essentially train themselves, and few occupations are 
  beyond their reach mentally. Persons of average IQ (between 90 and 
  110) are not competitive for most professional and executive-level 
  work but are easily trained for the bulk of jobs in the American 
  economy. In contrast, adults in the bottom 5 percent of the IQ 
  distribution (below 75) are very difficult to train and are not 
  competitive for any occupation on the basis of ability. Serious 
  problems in training low-IQ military recruits during World War II led 
  Congress to ban enlistment from the lowest 10 percent (below 80) of 
  the population, and no civilian occupation in modern economies 
  routinely recruits its workers from that range. Current military 
  enlistment standards exclude any individual whose IQ is below about 
  85.

  The importance of g in job performance, as in schooling, is related 
  to complexity. Occupations differ considerably in the complexity of 
  their demands, and as that complexity rises, higher g levels become a 
  bigger asset and lower g levels a bigger handicap. Similarly, 
  everyday tasks and environments also differ significantly in their 
  cognitive complexity. The degree to which a person's g level will 
  come to bear on daily life depends on how much novelty and ambiguity 
  that person's everyday tasks and surroundings present and how much 
  continual learning, judgment and decision making they require. As 
  gamblers, employers and bankers know, even marginal differences in 
  rates of return will yield big gains--or losses--over time. Hence, 
  even small differences in g among people can exert large, cumulative 
  influences across social and economic life.

  In my own work, I have tried to synthesize the many lines of research 
  that document the influence of IQ on life outcomes. As the 
  illustration shows, the odds of various kinds of achievement and 
  social pathology change systematically across the IQ continuum, from 
  borderline mentally retarded (below 70) to intellectually gifted 
  (above 130). Even in comparisons of those of somewhat below average 
  (between 76 and 90) and somewhat above average (between 111 and 125) 
  IQs, the odds for outcomes having social consequence are stacked 
  against the less able. Young men somewhat below average in general 
  mental ability, for example, are more likely to be unemployed than 
  men somewhat above average. The lower-IQ woman is four times more 
  likely to bear illegitimate children than the higher-IQ woman; among 
  mothers, she is eight times more likely to become a chronic welfare 
  recipient. People somewhat below average are 88 times more likely to 
  drop out of high school, seven times more likely to be jailed and 
  five times more likely as adults to live in poverty than people of 
  somewhat above-average IQ. Below-average individuals are 50 percent 
  more likely to be divorced than those in the above-average category.

  These odds diverge even more sharply for people with bigger gaps in 
  IQ, and the mechanisms by which IQ creates this divergence are not 
  yet clearly understood. But no other single trait or circumstance yet 
  studied is so deeply implicated in the nexus of bad social 
  outcomes--poverty, welfare, illegitimacy and educational 
  failure--that entraps many low-IQ individuals and families. Even the 
  effects of family background pale in comparison with the influence of 
  IQ. As shown most recently by Charles Murray of the American 
  Enterprise Institute in Washington, D.C., the divergence in many 
  outcomes associated with IQ level is almost as wide among siblings 
  from the same household as it is for strangers of comparable IQ 
  levels. And siblings differ a lot in IQ--on average, by 12 points, 
  compared with 17 for random strangers.

  An IQ of 75 is perhaps the most important threshold in modern life. 
  At that level, a person's chances of mastering the elementary school 
  curriculum are only 50-50, and he or she will have a hard time 
  functioning independently without considerable social support. 
  Individuals and families who are only somewhat below average in IQ 
  face risks of social pathology that, while lower, are still 
  significant enough to jeopardize their well-being. High-IQ 
  individuals may lack the resolve, character or good fortune to 
  capitalize on their intellectual capabilities, but socioeconomic 
  success in the postindustrial information age is theirs to lose.

  What Is versus What Could Be

  The foregoing findings on g's effects have been drawn from studies 
  conducted under a limited range of circumstances--namely, the social, 
  economic and political conditions prevailing now and in recent 
  decades in developed countries that allow considerable personal 
  freedom. It is not clear whether these findings apply to populations 
  around the world, to the extremely advantaged and disadvantaged in 
  the developing world or, for that matter, to people living under 
  restrictive political regimes. No one knows what research under 
  different circumstances, in different eras or with different 
  populations might reveal.

  But we do know that, wherever freedom and technology advance, life is 
  an uphill battle for people who are below average in proficiency at 
  learning, solving problems and mastering complexity. We also know 
  that the trajectories of mental development are not easily deflected. 
  Individual IQ levels tend to remain unchanged from adolescence 
  onward, and despite strenuous efforts over the past half a century, 
  attempts to raise g permanently through adoption or educational means 
  have failed. If there is a reliable, ethical way to raise or equalize 
  levels of g, no one has found it.

  Some investigators have suggested that biological interventions, such 
  as dietary supplements of vitamins, may be more effective than 
  educational ones in raising g levels. This approach is based in part 
  on the assumption that improved nutrition has caused the puzzling 
  rise in average levels of both IQ and height in the developed world 
  during this century. Scientists are still hotly debating whether the 
  gains in IQ actually reflect a rise in g or are caused instead by 
  changes in less critical, specific mental skills. Whatever the truth 
  may be, the differences in mental ability among individuals remain, 
  and the conflict between equal opportunity and equal outcome 
  persists. Only by accepting these hard truths about intelligence will 
  society find humane solutions to the problems posed by the variations 
  in general mental ability.


  Related Links

  IQ Tests on the WWW: Web Directory

  Intelligence and Personality Assessment: A Study Guide by Jon Potter

  IQ: A Structure for Understanding by Timothy Bates, Macquarie 
  University Sydney

  Great Ideas in Personality -- Intelligence by G. Scott Acton, 
  Northwestern University

  Intelligence and IQ: Book reviews, commentaries and links to other 
  Net resources.


  The Author

  LINDA S. GOTTFREDSON is professor of educational studies at the 
  University of Delaware, where she has been since 1986, and co-directs 
  the Delaware-Johns Hopkins Project for the Study of Intelligence and 
  Society. She trained as a sociologist, and her earliest work focused 
  on career development. "I wasn't interested in intelligence per se," 
  Gottfredson says. "But it suffused everything I was studying in my 
  attempts to understand who was getting ahead." This "discovery of the 
  obvious," as she puts it, became the focus of her research. In the 
  mid-1980s, while at Johns Hopkins University, she published several 
  influential articles describing how intelligence shapes vocational 
  choice and self-perception. Gottfredson also organized the 1994 
  treatise "Mainstream Science on Intelligence," an editorial with more 
  than 50 signatories that first appeared in the Wall Street Journal in 
  response to the controversy surrounding publication of The Bell 
  Curve. Gottfredson is the mother of identical twins--a "mere 
  coincidence," she says, "that's always made me think more about the 
  nature and nurture of intelligence." The girls, now 16, follow 
  Gottfredson's Peace Corps experience of the 1970s by joining her each 
  summer for volunteer construction work in the villages of Nicaragua.

------------------------------


** FOR YOUR REFERENCE **

The service address, to which questions about the list itself and requests
to be added to or deleted from it should be directed, is:

    Internet: [EMAIL PROTECTED]

You can send mail to the entire list by posting to comp.os.linux.advocacy.

Linux may be obtained via one of these FTP sites:
    ftp.funet.fi                                pub/Linux
    tsx-11.mit.edu                              pub/linux
    sunsite.unc.edu                             pub/Linux

End of Linux-Advocacy Digest
******************************

Reply via email to